Primate Freedom Project - Education, Advocacy, Support
Primate Freedom Project - Education, Advocacy, Support
LIFE STORIES
These are life stories of primates held in U.S. primate laboratories. They are based on documents obtained from the labs.
YNPRC
Clint Chimpanzee
Dover Chimpanzee
Sellers Chimpanzee
Tottie Chimpanzee
3566 Rhesus Macaque
PWc2 Rhesus Macaque
Unknown Rhesus Macaque
YN70-119 Chimpanzee
YN73-125 Gorilla
YN74-17 Chimpanzee
YN74-68 Chimpanzee
YN78-109 Chimpanzee
YN79-33 Chimpanzee
YN81-124 Chimpanzee
YN86-37 Squirrel Monkey
ONPRC
13447 Rhesus Macaque
13481 Rhesus Macaque
14326 Rhesus Macaque
20213 Rhesus Macaque
20229 Rhesus Macaque D
20233 Rhesus Macaque
20247 Rhesus Macaque
20253 Rhesus Macaque
20346 Rhesus Macaque
CNPRC
18714 Crab-eating Macaque
20629 Rhesus Macaque
22114 Crab-eating Macaque
23915 Crab-eating Macaque
23954 Squirrel Monkey
23993 Squirrel Monkey
23997 Squirrel Monkey
24005 Squirrel Monkey
24013 Squirrel Monkey
24557 Crab-eating Macaque
24605 Crab-eating Macaque
24974 Rhesus Macaque
24994 Rhesus Macaque
25142 Crab-eating Macaque
25157 Crab-eating Macaque
25205 Crab-eating Macaque
25250 Crab-eating Macaque
25274 Rhesus Macaque
25281 Rhesus Macaque
25412 Crab-eating Macaque
25809 Squirrel Monkey
27276 Crab-eating Macaque
27306 Rhesus Macaque
28092 Crab-eating Macaque
28098 Crab-eating Macaque
28100 Crab-eating Macaque
28104 Crab-eating Macaque
28109 Crab-eating Macaque
28114 Crab-eating Macaque
28545 Squirrel Monkey
28562 Squirrel Monkey
28796 Crab-eating Macaque
30749 Crab-eating Macaque
30755 Crab-eating Macaque
30813 Rhesus Macaque
30914 Rhesus Macaque
30916 Rhesus Macaque
30983 Rhesus Macaque
31031 Rhesus Macaque
34273 Crab-eating Macaque
34274 Crab-eating Macaque
34275 Crab-eating Macaque
34276 Crab-eating Macaque
34278 Crab-eating Macaque
34279 Crab-eating Macaque
34280 Crab-eating Macaque
34281 Crab-eating Macaque
WNPRC
cj0233 Common Marmoset
cj0453 Common Marmoset D
cj0495 Common Marmoset
cj0506 Common Marmoset
cj1654 Common Marmoset
Piotr Rhesus Macaque
rhaf72 Rhesus Macaque
rhao45 Rhesus Macaque
Rh1890 Rhesus Macaque
R80180 Rhesus Macaque
R87083 Rhesus Macaque
R89124 Rhesus Macaque
R89163 Rhesus Macaque
R90128 Rhesus Macaque
R91040 Rhesus Macaque
R93014 Rhesus Macaque
S93052 Rhesus Macaque
R95054 Rhesus Macaque D
R95065 Rhesus Macaque D
R95076 Rhesus Macaque D
R95100 Rhesus Macaque
R96108 Rhesus Macaque
R97041 Rhesus Macaque
R97082 Rhesus Macaque
R97111 Rhesus Macaque
Response from Jordana Lenon, public relations manager for WNPRC. Citizens' requests Lenon refused to answer.
WANPRC
A03068 Rhesus Macaque
A98056 Pig-tailed Macaque
A92025 Baboon
F91396 Pig-tailed Macaque D
J90153 Pig-tailed Macaque
J90266 Pig-tailed Macaque
J90299 Crab-eating Macaque
J91076 Pig-tailed Macaque D
J91386 Pig-tailed Macaque D
J91398 Pig-tailed Macaque D
J92068 Pig-tailed Macaque
J92349 Pig-tailed Macaque D
J92476 Pig-tailed Macaque
UCLA
B15A Vervet
788E Rhesus Macaque
9382 Vervet
1984-016 Vervet
1991-016 Vervet
1992-015 Vervet
1994-014 Vervet
1994-046 Vervet
1994-087 Vervet
1995-046 Vervet
1995-101 Vervet
1996-022 Vervet
UTAH
MCY24525 Crab-eating Macaque
MCY24540 Crab-eating Macaque
OIPM-007 Crab-eating Macaque
MCY24525 Crab-eating Macaque
MCY24540 Crab-eating Macaque
UNC-Chapel Hill
3710 Squirrel Monkey
APF
Ashley Chimpanzee
Karla Chimpanzee
Tyson Chimpanzee
Snoy Chimpanzee
Maurice p1 Maurice p2 Chimpanzee
Hercules Chimpanzee
Jerome Chimpanzee
Ritchie Chimpanzee
Rex Chimpanzee
Topsey Chimpanzee
B.G. Chimpanzee
Dawn Chimpanzee
BamBam Chimpanzee
Dixie Chimpanzee
Ginger Chimpanzee
Kelly Chimpanzee
Lennie Chimpanzee
Kist Chimpanzee
Peg Chimpanzee
Aaron Chimpanzee
Chuck Chimpanzee
James Chimpanzee
Alex Chimpanzee
Muna Chimpanzee
Wally Chimpanzee
#1028 Chimpanzee
Lippy Chimpanzee
#1303 Chimpanzee
#CA0127 Chimpanzee
Shane Chimpanzee
LEMSIP
196 Baboon
The Fauna Foundation Chimpanzees
Center for Biologics Evaluation
Univ. of Alabama - Birmingham

Univ. of Minnesota

00FP8 Long-tailed Macaque
312E Rhesus Macaque
9711B Rhesus Macaque
99IP61 Long-tailed Macaque
CDC-Column E 2002

 

Dora E. Angelaki:
the “Little Angel of St. Louis”1

Phone: 314-747-5528
Fax: 314-747-4370
2 East McDonnell SRF
Campus Box: 8108
Washington University School of Medicine
St. Louis, Missouri

angelaki@pcg.wustl.edu


Within less than an hour after an animal was euthanized and perfused, the head was put into the superstructure in exactly the same position
and was mounted exactly the same way as during experiments.

 

THE FOLLOWING STORY IS TRUE. IT IS NOT UNUSUAL.
IT IS REPRESENTATIVE OF THE CURRENT STATE OF RESEARCH IN THE UNITED STATES USING ANIMALS. WHAT THE STORY OF DORA E. ANGELAKI MAKES CLEAR, IS THAT ANY PRETENSE OF MEANINGFUL OVERSIGHT OR CLOSE SCRUTINY OF ANIMAL-BASED RESEARCH IN THE U.S. IS SIMPLE POLITICALLY CORRECT RHETORIC; RHETORIC INTENDED TO MOLLIFY AND SOOTHE THE SENSIBILITIES OF A PUBLIC PAYING SCANT ATTENTION TO WHAT IS ACTUALLY TAKING PLACE IN THE LABORATORIES BEING FUNDED WITH U.S. TAX MONIES.

THE STORY OF DORA E. ANGELAKI MAKES THIS CRYSTAL CLEAR.

The Little Angel of St. Louis has been publishing details of her daily activities since 1991. The Little Angel of St. Louis has written and published at least fifty-five articles that fit together as pieces of a puzzle to suggest a picture of a career, a life, an ethos and a woman. The Little Angel of St. Louis is a portrait of the full ascension to equality for one woman involved in the pursuit of knowledge for knowledge sake. It has taken bravery and an unflinching callousness in the face of unspeakable suffering.

In 1991, Dora E Angelaki published “Changes in the Dynamics of the Vertical Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex Due to Linear Acceleration in the Frontal Plane of the Cat.”2 Here, the Little Angel of St. Louis described an early experiment of a type that has remained her passion even to today. “The vertical and horizontal components of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) were recorded in alert, restrained cats who were placed on their sides and subjected to whole-body rotations in the horizontal plane. The head was either on the axis or 45 cm eccentric from the axis rotation.” She spun the cats with the lights on and in the dark.

During 1991, the Little Angel of St. Louis published three separate articles,3, 4 detailing the results of spinning cats in various ways. We can imagine the Little Angel as a child twisting in a swing and spinning around and around until so dizzy she could neither see nor walk straight. Maybe these experiences contributed to her passion for spinning and shaking animals. Also in 1991, Angelaki published details of her experiments spinning anesthetized gerbils.5 She discovered that the inner ears of gerbils and the inner ears of rats are different.

1992 was a very productive year for the Little Angel of St. Louis. She published nine papers in all. In “A Model for the Characterization of the Spatial Properties in Vestibular Neurons,”6 she argued that the use of rats whose brains had been disconnected from their spines were rewarding experimental tools. “In this paper, extracellular recordings from otolith-sensitive vestibular nuclei neurons in decerebrate rats were used to demonstrate the practical application of the method.” It was also in 1992 that the Little Angel of St. Louis first exhibited her interest in “wobbly eye” or nystagmus.7

In 1993 the Little Angel of St. Louis managed to get eight more papers published. 1993 was also the first year she began publishing the results of her experiments on monkeys.8 She demonstrated that when rhesus monkeys are spun and shaken in just the right manner that their eyes wobble.

Sinusoidal oscillation of rhesus monkeys about a head-fixed, earth-horizontal axis while rotating at constant velocity about an earth-vertical axis generates a characteristic ocular nystagmus where the three-dimensional slow phase eye velocity is compensatory to the spatially and temporally changing head angular velocity vector. This includes the generation of a unidirectional nystagmus characterised by a "bias" slow phase velocity component, albeit of small gain (0.2-0.7), that persists for the duration of the combined two-axes stimulation.

She also demonstrated that she could interfere with the normal ability of squirrel monkeys to compensate while being spun and keeping their eyes on a target, by turning the lights out and mildly shocking their inner ears.9 The Little Angel of St. Louis continued to experiment on decerebrate rats.10 "Decerebrate" describes animals whose cerebral brain functions have been eliminated for experimental purposes by cutting their brain stem or by other techniques.

1994 was less productive for the Little Angel, but she did manage to get two papers published. In one,11 she spins rhesus monkeys in almost every conceivable manner (almost, because the Little Angel of St. Louis is very imaginative and, as we shall see, is constantly inventing new ways to spin and shake animals.)

The spatial organization of the vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) was studied in six rhesus monkeys by applying fast, short-lasting, passive head and body tilts immediately after constant-velocity rotation (+/- 90 degrees/s) about an earth-vertical axis…Horizontal VOR was studied with the monkeys upright….Torsional VOR was studied with the monkeys in supine or prone position.

The Little Angel of St. Louis began surgically modifying the monkeys she experimented on in 1994 as well. “The vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) was investigated in rhesus monkeys before and after surgical ablation of the cerebellar nodulus and ventral uvula.”12 The cerebellar nodulus and ventral uvula are parts of the brain. The Little Angel discovered that monkeys who had these brain structures cut out did not develop the same wobbly eyes after being spun and shaken as did monkeys whose brains were not chopped up. They also had a hard time keeping their eye on a specific target as they were being spun this way and that.

In 1995 the Little Angel continued her investigations into the effects of spinning monkeys every which way after she had damaged parts of their brains. “Here we report that surgical inactivation of the cerebellar nodulus and ventral uvula abolished the ability of the otolith system to generate steady-state nystagmus during constant velocity rotation and to improve the dynamics of the vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) during low-frequency sinusoidal oscillations about off-vertical axes in rhesus monkeys.”13

There are three semicircular tubes in the bony labyrinth of the inner ear. They are concerned with balance and our ability to tell whether we are standing straight or leaning in some direction. In 1995, Angelaki first reported on her experiments plugging these canals in monkeys’ inner ears and then spinning them.14 What she discovered was that monkeys with the inner ear plugs also had wobbly eyes after being spun in various directions, whereas monkeys whose brains she had damaged and spun in the same ways sometimes didn’t have wobbly eyes. At the end of 1995, the Little Angel of St. Louis was able to announce, based on her nearly five years of spinning various species and experimental brain surgeries that “Inertial vestibular signals are likely to contribute to head control and motor coordination of gaze, head and body posture.”15

1996 was a more productive year for the Little Angel. Her explanations also started becoming more detailed. She was working in Jackson, Mississippi at the University of Mississippi Medical Center at the time, and we must imagine that the continued support by the university gave her a confidence to try ever new and more abstract ways of spinning animals. It seemed that every time she could contrive of a novelty, there was always a scientific journal ready to publish the results from her new experiments. The university must have felt the prestige associated with having a scientist of the Little Angel’s caliber on their staff.

A common theme in Angelaki’s experiments had become “sinusoidal linear acceleration.” A sine wave is shaped like an ‘s’ lying on its side. So the monkeys experiencing “sinusoidal linear acceleration” were being accelerated in a straight line while being moved up and down – like being on a roller coaster without curves.

But this wasn’t all, as the monkeys were being accelerated in this manner, the Little Angel of St. Louis also spun them this way and that. And sometimes, all of this took place in the dark. Maybe the Little Angel had a secret unfulfilled longing to work in a carnival.

The dynamic properties of otolith-ocular reflexes elicited by sinusoidal linear acceleration along the three cardinal head axes were studied during off-vertical axis rotations in rhesus monkeys. As the head rotates in space at constant velocity about an off-vertical axis, otolith-ocular reflexes are elicited in response to the sinusoidally varying linear acceleration (gravity) components along the interaural, nasooccipital, or vertical head axis…. Animals were rotated in complete darkness in the yaw, pitch, and roll planes at velocities ranging between 7.4 and 184 degrees/s.16

More variation was needed in order to keep publishing. “In an attempt to separate response components to head velocity from those to head position relative to gravity during low-frequency sinusoidal oscillations, large oscillation amplitudes were chosen such that peak-to-peak head displacements exceeded 360 degrees.”17

And she continued to plug ear canals:

The ability of the vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) to undergo adaptive modification after selective changes in the peripheral vestibular system was investigated in rhesus monkeys by recording three-dimensional eye movements before and after inactivation of selective semicircular canals…. The spatial organization of the VOR was investigated during oscillations at different head positions in the pitch, roll, and yaw planes, as well as in the right anterior/left posterior and left anterior/right posterior canal planes. Acutely after bilateral inactivation of the lateral semicircular canals, a small horizontal response could still be elicited that peaked during rotations in pitched head positions that would maximally stimulate vertical semicircular canals….18

By 1997, the Little Angel of St. Louis was in her full stride:

This study presents data obtained from five juvenile rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) that were prepared chronically with a scleral dual-search coil for three-dimensional eye movement recording and head bolts for restraining the head during the experiments. … During the experiments, animals were seated in a primate chair with the head restrained in a position of 15° nose-down relative to the stereotaxic horizontal (defined as "upright" position) to place the lateral semicircular canals approximately earth horizontal…
The animals were placed inside the inner frame of a multiaxis turntable with three motor-driven gimbaled axes. The effect of dynamic changes in head orientation relative to gravity on fast and slow eye movements was studied during either constant-velocity rotation or sinusoidal oscillations of the animals about their head-vertical (yaw) axis, which was oriented in the earth-horizontal plane (90° off-vertical).19

During 1997, the Little Angel returned briefly to her experiments on pigeons.20

The papers authored by Angelaki rarely mention a possible health need associated with the experiments she has and is conducting. She does not mention how her work might be useful in combating disease nor does she suggest that the results might offer insight into a human malady. Angelaki is interested in mathematical formulae that might model the vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) and the adaptive modifications in the animal’s system following experimental injury. It seems not unreasonable to ask what might bring someone to the point in their life that they would not only contemplate performing such experiments, but actually carry them out, repetitively. Nor is it unreasonable to ask, how the universities’ oversight committees could have allowed such experiments to have continued on and on with no pretense of medical importance. (The lack of meaningful oversight by the universities’ oversight committees appears to be the norm.)21

It is reasonable to fault Angelaki directly; she is responsible for doing what she has done. It is unreasonable to assign her sole blame. The U.S. government has funded Angelaki’s work. This means that many people share equally in the responsibility for Angelaki’s research. The system of checks and balances does not work. We might wish to believe that Angelaki is an anomaly of some sort, that she is performing her nightmarish investigations at some out-of-the-way backwater, or maybe she is actually at a lab in a facility so large that she has gotten lost in the shuffle.

The Little Angel has been affiliated with many institutions. They all sanctioned her work. Many people know what she does, her work is published in publicly accessible, if obscure, scientific journals. We should ask: Why does American society allow and pay to have such experiments performed? What has become of a society that has institutions that condone and nurture scientists who wish to perform such experiments? What has become of a society where citizens who hear of such atrocity turn away? What of a legal system that would protect such behavior? What of a society that would elect politicians who would provide public support for such experiments?

Something has gone wrong.

In 1991 the Little Angel of St. Louis was affiliated with the University of Texas Medical Branch, Galveston. In 1992 the Little Angel of St. Louis was involved with the Department of Physiology at the University of Minnesota, Minneapolis and the Department of Neurology at the University of Zurich, Switzerland. By 1996, the Little Angel was with the Department of Surgery (Otolaryngology) at the University of Mississippi Medical Center, Jackson.

In 1998, Angelaki explained that her methods were within the accepted norm.

Seven rhesus monkeys were chronically prepared with skull bolts to restrain the head during experiments and implanted with a dual search coil for three-dimensional eye movement recordings using the magnetic search coil technique. Of these, five animals were used for control responses. In addition, data were also collected from five animals after selective semicircular canals were inactivated by plugging. The lateral canals were plugged in two (LC) animals, the right anterior/left posterior canals were plugged in another two (RALP) animals, and all canals were plugged in the fifth animal…. The efficacy of canal-plugging has been histologically verified…. All surgeries were performed under intubation anesthesia, and animal treatment and handling was in accordance with the National Institutes of Health guidelines.

For experiments, the monkeys were seated in a primate chair with their heads restrained in a position such that the horizontal stereotaxic plane was tilted15° nose-down. In this head position, the vertical semicircular canals were approximately perpendicular and the lateral semicircular canals approximately parallel to the earth-horizontal plane. The monkeys were placed in a primate chair that was secured inside a motorized three-dimensional turntable that could deliver both earth-vertical and -horizontal axis rotations about the yaw, pitch, and roll axes. The turntable was surrounded completely by a light-tight sphere (80 cm radius) covered with a random dot pattern such that eye movements could be studied in complete darkness (when the lights inside the sphere were off). This sphere also could be oscillated independently such that horizontal or vertical (but not torsional) optokinetic optic flow could be generated (with the lights inside the sphere on).

Before experimental sessions, animals were given a small dose of d-amphetamine (1.5 mg orally) to maintain a constant level of alertness. Monkeys were subjected to 2 h of simultaneous vestibular and optokinetic oscillations at each of two frequencies….22

This gives us a clear indication of what National Institutes of Health guidelines allow to occur.

Also, in 1998, the Little Angel of St. Louis published “Three-Dimensional Organization of Otolith-Ocular Reflexes in Rhesus Monkeys. III. Responses to Translation.”23 It seems she was willing to perform ever more bizarre and questionable experimental surgeries to prepare her experimental victims.

In this paper she introduced the novelty of tilting the monkeys heads at 18° nose down, as opposed to her previous 15° nose-down regime. Regarding the 15° she had said, “In this head position, the vertical semicircular canals were approximately perpendicular and the lateral semicircular canals approximately parallel to the earth-horizontal plane.” Regarding the 18° she now said, “This head position was used to place the lateral semicircular canals approximately parallel to the earth-horizontal plane, whereas at the same time keeping the vertical semicircular canals as vertically oriented as possible.”

Five young rhesus monkeys (3-4 kg) were used in the present studies. Each animal was chronically implanted with a delrin ring imbedded in dental acrylic that was anchored to the skull by six stainless steel screws that were inverted and placed into T-slots cut into the skull. The ring was lightweight but provided a strong head restraint for vigorous stimulus motion and was used extensively for similar types of experimentation. In separate surgical procedures, a dual search coil designed for recording 3-D eye movements was implanted on each eye under the conjunctiva at ~3-5 mm from the limbus cornea and anterior to all eye muscle insertions. The lead wires from the eye coil were passed out of the orbit, under the muscle and skin, to the top of the skull where they exited inside the delrin ring. A connector plug was soldered to the lead wires and secured to the head ring with dental acrylic. When the animals were in their cages, the implanted delrin ring was covered with a cap to protect the eye coil plugs. After control responses were collected, all six semicircular canals were inactivated in two animals by plugging the canal lumen. Canal-plugged animals showed no evidence of increased spontaneous nystagmus either acutely or chronically. Following the surgery, the animals were kept in complete darkness until the following morning when the animals were brought to the laboratory for vestibular testing ("acute" experimental protocol). After this acute VOR testing, the animals were returned to the regular, daily light-dark cycle. All surgical procedures were performed under sterile conditions in accordance with the NIH guidelines
During experimental testing, the monkeys were seated in a primate chair with their heads statically positioned such that the horizontal stereotaxic plane was tilted 18° nose down. This head position was used to place the lateral semicircular canals approximately parallel to the earth-horizontal plane, whereas at the same time keeping the vertical semicircular canals as vertically oriented as possible. The animal's body was secured with shoulder and lap belts, whereas the extremities were loosely tied to the chair. The primate chair was then secured inside the inner frame of a vestibular turntable consisting of a 3-D rotator on top of a linear sled (Acutronics). The two inner frames of the turntable were manufactured by nonmetalic composite materials to minimize interference with the magnetic fields. In addition, the whole rotator assembly was specially constructed to provide rigid coupling between the motion generator (in these experiments, the linear sled) and the animal. The linear sled (2-m length) was powered by a servo-controlled linear motor that could deliver steady-state sinusoidal stimulation in a large frequency range (0.16-25 Hz). Using the 3-D turntable, the animals were repositioned relative to the direction of translation such that translational VORs were recorded during lateral (i.e., along the interaural axis, with the animals either upright or supine), fore-aft (i.e., along the naso-occipital axis, with the animals upright), and up-down (i.e., along the vertical head- and body-axis, with the animals either supine or right ear down) motion. For the present experiments, eye movements were recorded in complete darkness. For this, the animal's chair was completely surrounded by a light-tight sphere (61-cm radius).

1999 was a productive year for the Little Angel. She succeeded in getting six papers published:

1. Oculomotor Control of Primary Eye Position Discriminates Between Translation and Tilt (Journal of Neurophysiology)

2. Short-Latency Primate Vestibuloocular Responses During Translation (Journal of Neurophysiology)

3. Computation of Inertial Motion: Neural Strategies to Resolve Ambiguous Otolith Information (Journal of Neuroscience)

4. Functional Organization of Primate Translational Vestibulo-Ocular Reflexes and Effects of Unilateral Labyrinthectomy (Annals of the New York Academy of Science)

5. Inertial Processing of Vestibulo-Ocular Signals (Annals of the New York Academy of Science)

6. Three-Dimensional Organization of Vestibular-Related Eye Movements to Off-Vertical Axis Rotation and Linear Translation in Pigeons (Experimental Brain Research)

In Occulomotor Control24 (1 above) the Little Angel described her newest innovation for shaking the monkeys:

Data were obtained from five juvenile rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), which were chronically prepared with scleral dual-search coils for three-dimensional eye movement recording and a delrin head ring for restraining the head during the experiments….During the experiments, animals were seated in a primate chair with the head restrained in a position of 18° nose-down relative to the stereotaxic horizontal (defined as "upright" position) to place the lateral semicircular canals approximately earth-horizontal. The animals were placed inside the inner frame of a superstructure consisting of two motor-driven gimbaled axes. The superstructure was mounted on a computer-controlled sled that moved on ball-bearings on a 2.0 m long earth-horizontal track

 

Short Latency25 (number 2 above) repeats, what has become for the Little Angel of St. Louis, the routine shaking and accelerating. The Little Angel, at this point in time, seemed to be on a plateau of experimental routine, with little novelty in her studies. She wrote:

 

Four juvenile rhesus monkeys were chronically implanted with a head restraint platform and dual coils on each eye. Binocular three-dimensional (3-D) eye movements were recorded inside a magnetic field (CNC Engineering), then calibrated and expressed as rotation vectors (relative to straight-ahead; leftward was positive). The motion was delivered by a whole-body displacement on a sled (Acutronics) either along the lateral or fore-aft direction. Translational stimuli consisted of a steplike linear acceleration profile, followed by a short period of constant velocity (peak linear acceleration: 0.5 G; steady-state velocity: ±22 cm/s). The stimulus waveform had a frequency content of <50 Hz.

2000 was a good year for the Little Angel. She published seven papers, though six of them were in the Journal of Neurophysiology. She published a four-part series of papers titled Primate Translational Vestibuloocular Reflexes. I, II, III and IV. In part I, High-Frequency Dynamics and Three-Dimensional Properties During Lateral Motion,26 Angelaki explained her techniques:

Five juvenile rhesus monkeys were chronically implanted with a circular molded, lightweight dental acrylic ring that was anchored to the skull by stainless steel screws (for more details, see Angelaki 1998). Dual eye coils designed for recording binocular 3-D eye movements were implanted under the conjunctiva at ~3-5 mm from the limbus corneae and anterior to all eye muscle insertions (Hess 1990). Coils were sutured securely to the globe with at least four silk stitches. The lead wires were passed out of the orbit, under the muscle and skin to the head holder where they were soldered to connectors and secured to the head ring with dental acrylic. When animals were in their cages, the implanted delrin ring was covered with a cap to protect the eye coil plugs. All surgical procedures were performed under sterile conditions in accordance to institutional guidelines….

The dual eye coil assembly that was implanted on each eye consisted of two serially interconnected miniature coils (Sokymat, Switzerland) that were attached at diagonal points along the circumference of a large three-turn coil (Cooner wire, ~15 mm diam). The exact orientation of the two coils relative to one another, as well as the orientation of the dual search coil on the eye were determined based on both preimplantation and daily calibration procedures Before implantation, each dual eye coil was calibrated using a calibration jig. Using rotations about all three axes, this calibration yielded the horizontal and vertical angular orientations of the two coil sensitivity vectors as well as the angle between them. Because of the stable geometry of the dual coil assembly, these parameters were assumed to remain unchanged before and after implantation. On each experimental session and before the experimental protocols, pretrained animals performed a visual fixation task (targets at a distance of 1.5 m)….

During experimental testing, the monkeys were seated in a primate chair with their heads statically positioned such that the horizontal stereotaxic plane was tilted 18° nose-down. The animal's body was secured with shoulder and lap belts, while the extremities were loosely restrained to the chair. The primate chair then was secured inside the inner frame of a vestibular turntable consisting of a 3-D rotator on top of a linear sled (Acutronics). The two inner frames of the turntable and the associated gimbal structures were manufactured by nonmetalic composite materials to minimize interference with the magnetic fields. In addition, the whole rotator assembly and gimbal structure were constructed specially to provide rigid coupling between the motion generator and the animal. For these experiments, animals were maintained upright and were translated laterally during stimulation. Before experimental sessions, animals were sometimes given a small dose of D-amphetamine (1.0 mg orally) to maintain a constant level of alertness.

All animals participating in these experiments were pretrained using juice rewards to fixate targets paired with auditory cues for variable time periods (300-1000 ms), then to maintain fixation after the target was turned off for as long as the auditory tone was present (1 s). During all fixations, the room was illuminated (through small red lights) such that the animals could easily establish relative distance estimates of the targets. Adequate fixation was defined when both eyes were within behavioral windows (separate for each eye) of less than ±1.0° (for far and near central targets) or ±2.0° (for near eccentric targets with eye position >20°). Usually animals were trained 5 days/wk with free access to water during the weekend.

During experimental testing, animals were oscillated sinusoidally at different frequencies…

Because the present study employed high-frequency stimulation, the possibility for artifact in the recordings has been a main concern. We have addressed this problem with the following steps. First, we constructed a high-rigidity gimbal and coil frames as well as head attachment couplings. Second, we monitored a head coil securely fastened on the animal's head and measured the elicited "eye" movements immediately after an animal had been euthanized. Each of these steps is described in more detail in the following text. Finally, we have limited our quantitative analyses to data where the estimated error in measurement was judged to be <10% (usually <5%).

Special care was taken to tightly and securely fasten the animal's head to the magnetic coils and to the stiff inner gimbal of the 3-D turntable. In addition, the eye coil leads were taped securely to the superstructure. The following control experiments were conducted to quantify the errors in our eye movement measurements.

To investigate the possibility that the head coil did not accurately reflect the movement of the head (e.g., through incomplete coupling or loose head-holder), the following test was performed. Within less than an hour after an animal was euthanized and perfused, the head was put into the superstructure in exactly the same position and was mounted exactly the same way as during experiments….

[This work was supported by grants from the National Eye Institute (EY-12814 and EY-10851), the Air Force Office of Scientific Research (F-49620), and the Swiss National Science Foundation (31-4728796) and by a Presidential Young Investigator Award for Scientists and Engineers (National Aeronautics and Space Administration NAG 5-3884).]

It is certainly a novel idea to cut a monkey’s head off and reattach it to the experimental apparatus and check to see how bounce inherent in the apparatus might be affecting the data. That Little Angel… always striving for the new and untested.

In Part II, Version and Vergence Responses to Fore-Aft Motion,27 Angelaki explains:

Nine juvenile rhesus monkeys provided the data presented here. Each animal was chronically implanted with a lightweight delrin head ring anchored to the skull with stainless steel screws and dental acrylic. Dual scleral eye coils were implanted in both eyes beneath the conjunctiva and sutured to the globe anterior to all muscular insertions. All surgical procedures were performed aseptically in accordance with National Institutes of Health guidelines.

In Part III, Effects of Bilateral Labyrinthine Electrical Stimulation,28 Angelaki writes:

Five juvenile rhesus monkeys were chronically implanted with a circular molded, light-weight dental acrylic ring that was anchored by stainless steel screws, placed as inverted T-bolts under the skull and then secured to the ring. For single-unit recordings from the vestibular nerve in three of the animals, a platform (3 cm × 3 cm, 5 mm height) constructed of machinable plastic-delrin was secured stereotaxically to the skull and fitted inside the head ring. The platform had staggered rows of holes (spaced 0.8 mm apart) that extended from the midline to the area overlying the vestibular nerves bilaterally.

Subsequent to the eye coil surgeries and after animals had been trained sufficiently to fixate visual targets, labyrinthine stimulating electrodes were implanted in both ears. An incision was made on the rear side of the pinna and the temporal bone exposed. The soft tissue of the external ear canal was displaced gently and the bony meatus enlarged using a dental drill until the long process of the malleus and the chorda tympani (facial nerve) were visualized. A platinized Teflon-insulated silver wire (250 µm diam and insulated to within 1 mm of its tip) then was press fit into a small hole drilled into the promontorium between the round and oval windows. The electrode penetrated into the perilymphatic space but was sealed against perilymphatic leak by the Teflon insulation. A second, reference electrode was placed into a hole drilled close to the entrance of the bony meatus. The two wires were led under the skin to the top of the skull and mated to a connector. The incision in the temporal muscle and the skin was sutured closed. When animals were in their cages, the implanted delrin ring was covered with a cap to protect the recording platform and prohibit the animals from touching the leads of the eye coils and stimulating electrodes.

…1) Three animals were sinusoidally laterally translated in complete darkness at several frequencies ranging between 0.3 and 12 Hz. At the lowest frequencies (0.3 and 0.37 Hz), the stimulus amplitude was 0.2 and 0.3 g, respectively. At higher frequencies, the amplitude was 0.3-0.4 g. To examine if the effects of the currents differed for different stimulus amplitudes, peak linear acceleration for 5-Hz oscillations was varied between 0.1 and 0.4 g in two animals.

2) Four animals were oscillated laterally at different frequencies between 4 and 12 Hz (0.3-0.4 g) while fixating on a centered (i.e., approximately zero horizontal eccentricity relative to a point midway between the two eyes) head-fixed target LED located 40, 30, 20, 15, or 10 cm from the eyes (in an otherwise dark laboratory room).

 

It should be noted, perhaps, that the unit of measure Hz is hertz. Hertz is a measure of cycles per second. Thus, when the Little Angel says she oscillated monkeys laterally at 12 Hz, what she is saying is that she shook them back and forth 12 times a second. Typically, in her studies, she maintains the oscillations for two hours at a time.

Part IV, Changes After Unilateral Labyrinthectomy,29 is essentially more of the same sort of experimentation – the shaking, accelerating, etc, but with the monkeys’ inner ear structure, the labyrinth, surgically damaged either in one ear or both. Some of the monkeys used in this series of experiments had been used previously in some of the semi-circular canal plugging experiments. “In two of the animals (B and E), the left labyrinth was destroyed. The other three animals (H, P, and R) underwent right labyrinthectomy. Animals B and R were labyrinthectomized 3-4 mo after all semicircular canals were inactivated as part of a different study. In animals E, H, and P, the semicircular canals were intact at the time of unilateral labyrinthectomy.”

Using animals in multiple survival surgeries is generally considered to be a violation of the federal Animal Welfare Act, but the sky was the limit for the Little Angel because she had finally secured her position in the Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology at Washington University School of Medicine in St. Louis, Missouri.

 

Washington University School of Medicine holds a rich history of success in research, education and patient care, earning it a reputation as one of the premier medical schools in the world. Since its founding in 1891, the School has trained nearly 6,000 physicians and has contributed ground-breaking discoveries in many areas of medical research.30

It would be unreasonable to assume that “one of the premier medical schools in the world” was not aware of the Little Angel’s published papers. One could wonder whether it was the severed monkeys’ heads spinning and being shaken in the Little Angel’s macabre machinery that appealed most to the selection committee at the Washington University School of Medicine. Perhaps it was the vigor of her publishing history – after all, claiming that a new faculty member has published forty-eight papers (at this point in her career) sounds impressive; and the likelihood that anyone would actually go out and read them, remote.

One has to wonder just what it was about the Little Angel that Washington University School of Medicine found so attractive. Her work has no pretense of applicability to human medicine. She is not claiming in her publications to be looking for a cure for deafness, a cure for nystagmus, vertigo, or motion sickness, though the monkeys she uses may well find the erratic motions she subjects them to sickening…. What could be behind a decision to bring someone such as the Little Angel and all their contraptions to one’s university? Perhaps we will never know.

What is certain, however, is that she has continued to find the same support at Washington University School of Medicine as she had at the University of Mississippi Medical Center. The fact that schools of medicine have supported, and continue to support the Little Angel’s work is a living and loud rebuttal to the claim that the research occurring in these institutions is intended to help humans, is carefully considered for the probability that it will yield benefit, or that the animal-use oversight committees, the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees in the vernacular of the Animal Welfare Act, are in the least iota, meaningful.

One way a scientist’s work can be judged is by the number of times a paper is cited by other scientists. This is considered a measure of noteworthiness. Important papers may be cited frequently and repeatedly in the literature. Papers cited rarely, or never, may be seen as unimportant to the rest of the community of science. For instance, Angelaki’s Three-Dimensional Organization of Otolith-Ocular Reflexes in Rhesus Monkeys. III. Responses to Translation (see note 23), published in 1998 has been cited eleven times, perhaps an impressive number considering the arcane nature of the Little Angel’s work. But, eight of these citations were Angelaki citing her own work. Many of her papers have been cited only once, and some by only herself or another of her co-authors. Essentially, no one in the scientific community is paying any attention to her work, or, if they are reading her studies at all, is judging them insufficient to draw upon.

In any event, the St. Louis research community continues to support and nurture the Little Angel. Since moving to the Washington University School of Medicine in St. Louis, Dora Angelaki has published an additional six papers, three more in 2000 and three, so far, in 2001. While there, she has published:

1. Low-Frequency Otolith and Semicircular Canal Interactions after Canal Inactivation (2000, Experimental Brain Research)

2. Spatiotemporal Processing of Linear Acceleration: Primary Afferent and Central Vestibular Neuron Responses (2000, Journal of Neurophysiology)

3. Central Versus Peripheral Origin of Vestibuloocular Reflex Recovery Following Semicircular Canal Plugging in Rhesus Monkeys (2000, Journal of Neurophysiology)

4. Differential Sensorimotor Processing of Vestibulo-Ocular Signals During Rotation and Translation (2001, Journal of Neuroscience)

5. Cross-Axis Adaptation of the Translational Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex (2001, Experimental Brain Research)

6. Head Unrestrained Horizontal Gaze Shifts after Unilateral Labyrinthectomy in the Rhesus Monkey (2001, Experimental Brain Research)

In Fiscal 2000, the Little Angel of St. Louis received public support for her research through two federal grants. Under one grant, 5 R01DC004260-02, Neural Mechanisms of Vestibular Adaptation, she received $219,951. This was awarded by the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, a part of the National Institutes of Health (NIH). This grant will continue to be funded at this annual rate through 2004. In her written justification for receiving these funds, Angelaki writes:

Changes in vestibular function through disease, trauma and aging occur frequently and are particularly pronounced with exposure to unusual motion or gravitational environments. Throughout the history of the manned space flight program, the introduction of the body into microgravity has produced vestibular-related disturbances that result in personal discomfort and a loss in crew performance. Since the symptoms subside within several days of microgravity exposure, it suggests that the vestibular system responses can adaptively change to altered sensory conditions. These changes may be similar to the process of vestibular compensation which is observed following unilateral labyrinthine loss or alterations in visual-vestibular interactions. In order to better understand the nature of vestibular adaptation and its effects upon motor function, the processes underlying neural plasticity and adaptation to altered vestibular signals must be established.31

For grant 5 R01EY012814-02, 3D Organization and Neural Plasticity of Primate VOR, she received $290,127 from the National Eye Institute, another tentacle of NIH. This grant will continue to be funded at a comparable annual rate through 2003.

Dora E. Angelaki, the Little Angel of St. Louis, will continue to perform her cruel and meaningless experiments on monkeys. She will continue to be paid to do this with money taken from taxpayers. The biomedical lobby will continue to defend every experiment performed on animals, no matter how absurd or cruel, no matter how meaningless or wasteful; that’s their job, and they pursue it with great zest and relish. Until the public speaks with a loud enough voice, the politicians with the power to end these horrors will not listen, they simply will not care.

Knowing now, as you do, what is happening in U.S. laboratories, you must become either an accomplice by remaining silent and doing nothing, or else, you must become actively involved somehow. By becoming involved, you will be branded a nut. Only nuts, apparently, care enough about torture to speak out against it.

Good luck.

Rick Bogle
September 5, 2001

Notes:

1. Angelaki: little angel (modern Greek)

2. Angelaki DE, Anderson JH, Blakley BW. Changes in the dynamics of the vertical
vestibulo-ocular reflex due to linear acceleration in the frontal plane of the cat.
Experimental Brain Research. 1991; 86(1):27-39.

3. Angelaki DE, Anderson JH. The horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex during linear
acceleration in the frontal plane of the cat. Experimental Brain Research. 1991;86(1):40-6.

4. Angelaki DE, Anderson JH. The vestibulo-ocular reflex in the cat during linear
acceleration in the sagittal plane. Brain Research. 1991 Mar 15; 543(2):347-50.

5. Dickman JD, Angelaki DE, Correia MJ. Response properties of gerbil otolith afferents
to small angle pitch and roll tilts. Brain Research. 1991 Aug 16;556(2):303-10.

6. Angelaki DE, Bush GA, Perachio AA. A model for the characterization of the spatial
properties in vestibular neurons. Biological Cybernetics. 1992;66(3):231-40.

7. Angelaki DE, Perachio AA, Mustari MJ, Strunk CL. Role of irregular otolith afferents
in the steady-state nystagmus during off-vertical axis rotation. Journal of Neurophysiology.
1992 Nov; 68(5):1895-900.

8. Hess BJ, Angelaki DE. Angular velocity detection by head movements orthogonal to
the plane of rotation. Experimental Brain Research. 1993; 95(1):77-83.

9. Angelaki DE, Perachio AA. Contribution of irregular semicircular canal afferents to
the horizontal vestibuloocular response during constant velocity rotation. Journal of
Neurophysiology. 1993 Mar; 69(3):996-9.

10. Angelaki DE, Bush GA, Perachio AA. Two-dimensional spatiotemporal coding of
linear acceleration in vestibular nuclei neurons. Journal of Neuroscience.
1993 Apr;13(4):1403-17.

11. Angelaki DE, Hess BJ. Inertial representation of angular motion in the vestibular
system of rhesus monkeys. I. Vestibuloocular reflex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1994
Mar; 71(3):1222-49.

12. Angelaki DE, Hess BJ. The cerebellar nodulus and ventral uvula control the torsional
vestibulo-ocular reflex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1994 Sep;72(3):1443-7.

13. Angelaki DE, Hess BJ. Lesion of the nodulus and ventral uvula abolish steady-state
off-vertical axis otolith response. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1995 Apr;73(4):1716-20.

14. Angelaki DE, Hess BJ. Inertial representation of angular motion in the vestibular
system of rhesus monkeys. II. Otolith-controlled transformation that depends on an intact
cerebellar nodulus. Journal of Neurophysiol. 1995 May; 73(5):1729-51.

15. Angelaki DE, Hess BJ, Suzuki J. Differential processing of semicircular canal signals
in the vestibulo-ocular reflex. Journal of Neuroscience. 1995 Nov;15(11):7201-16.

16. Angelaki DE, Hess BJ. Three-dimensional organization of otolith-ocular reflexes in
rhesus monkeys. I. Linear acceleration responses during off-vertical axis rotation. Journal
of Neurophysiology. 1996 Jun;75(6):2405-24.

17. Angelaki DE, Hess BJ. Three-dimensional organization of otolith-ocular reflexes in
rhesus monkeys. II. Inertial detection of angular velocity. Journal of Neurophysiology.
1996 Jun; 75(6):2425-40.

18. Angelaki DE, Hess BJ. Adaptation of primate vestibuloocular reflex to altered
peripheral vestibular inputs. II Spatiotemporal properties of the adapted slow-phase eye
velocity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1996 Nov;76(5):2954-71.

19. Hess BJ, Angelaki DE. Kinematic principles of primate rotational vestibulo-ocular
reflex. II. Gravity-dependent modulation of primary eye position. Journal of Neurophysiology.
1997 Oct; 78(4):2203-16.

20. Si X, Angelaki DE, Dickman JD. Response properties of pigeon otolith afferents to
linear acceleration. Experimental Brain Research. 1997 Nov;117(2):242-50.

21. Plous S, Herzog H. Animal research: reliability of protocol reviews for animal research.
Science. 2001 Jul; 298(5530): 608-609.

22. Angelaki DE, Hess BJ. Visually induced adaptation in three-dimensional organization
of primate vestibuloocular reflex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998 Feb;79(2):791-807.

23. Angelaki DE. Three-dimensional organization of otolith-ocular reflexes in rhesus
monkeys. III. Responses To translation. Journal of Neurophysiology.
1998 Aug; 80(2):680-95.

24. Hess BJ, Angelaki DE. Oculomotor control of primary eye position discriminates
between translation and tilt. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1999 Jan;81(1):394-8.

25. Angelaki DE, McHenry MQ. Short-latency primate vestibuloocular responses during
translation. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1999 Sep; 82(3):1651-4

26. Angelaki DE, McHenry MQ, Hess BJ. Primate translational vestibuloocular reflexes. I.
High-frequency dynamics and three-dimensional properties during lateral motion.
Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000 Mar;83(3):1637-47.

27. McHenry MQ, Angelaki DE. Primate translational vestibuloocular reflexes. II.
Version and vergence responses to fore-aft motion. Journal of Neurophysiology.
2000 Mar; 83(3):1648-61.

28. Angelaki DE, McHenry MQ, Dickman JD, Perachio AA. Primate translational
vestibuloocular reflexes. III. Effects of bilateral labyrinthine electrical stimulation.
Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000 Mar; 83(3):1662-76.

29. Angelaki DE, Newlands SD, Dickman JD. Primate translational vestibuloocular
reflexes. IV. Changes after unilateral labyrinthectomy. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000
May; 83(5):3005-18.

30 From Washington University School of Medicine's home page
http://medicine.wustl.edu/.

31. CRISP (Computer Retrieval of Information on Scientific Projects) Office of
Extramural Research at the National Institutes of Health.


Primate Freedom Project
P.O. Box 1623
Fayetteville, GA. 30214
Tel: 678.489.7798

Email: info@primatefreedom.com


Home Page | Our Mission | News
What Are Primate Freedom Tags | Order Tag
Primate Research Centers | Resources