Primate Freedom Project - Education, Advocacy, Support Primate Freedom Project - Education, Advocacy, Support
LIFE STORIES
These are life stories of primates held in U.S. primate laboratories. They are based on documents obtained from the labs.
YNPRC
Clint Chimpanzee
Dover Chimpanzee
Sellers Chimpanzee
Tottie Chimpanzee
3566 Rhesus Macaque
PWc2 Rhesus Macaque
Unknown Rhesus Macaque
YN70-119 Chimpanzee
YN73-125 Gorilla
YN74-17 Chimpanzee
YN74-68 Chimpanzee
YN78-109 Chimpanzee
YN79-33 Chimpanzee
YN81-124 Chimpanzee
YN86-37 Squirrel Monkey
ONPRC
13447 Rhesus Macaque
13481 Rhesus Macaque
14326 Rhesus Macaque
20213 Rhesus Macaque
20229 Rhesus Macaque D
20233 Rhesus Macaque
20247 Rhesus Macaque
20253 Rhesus Macaque
20346 Rhesus Macaque
CNPRC
18714 Crab-eating Macaque
20629 Rhesus Macaque
22114 Crab-eating Macaque
23915 Crab-eating Macaque
23954 Squirrel Monkey
23993 Squirrel Monkey
23997 Squirrel Monkey
24005 Squirrel Monkey
24013 Squirrel Monkey
24557 Crab-eating Macaque
24605 Crab-eating Macaque
24974 Rhesus Macaque
24994 Rhesus Macaque
25142 Crab-eating Macaque
25157 Crab-eating Macaque
25205 Crab-eating Macaque
25250 Crab-eating Macaque
25274 Rhesus Macaque
25281 Rhesus Macaque
25412 Crab-eating Macaque
25809 Squirrel Monkey
27276 Crab-eating Macaque
27306 Rhesus Macaque
28092 Crab-eating Macaque
28098 Crab-eating Macaque
28100 Crab-eating Macaque
28104 Crab-eating Macaque
28109 Crab-eating Macaque
28114 Crab-eating Macaque
28545 Squirrel Monkey
28562 Squirrel Monkey
28796 Crab-eating Macaque
30749 Crab-eating Macaque
30755 Crab-eating Macaque
30813 Rhesus Macaque
30914 Rhesus Macaque
30916 Rhesus Macaque
30983 Rhesus Macaque
31031 Rhesus Macaque
34273 Crab-eating Macaque
34274 Crab-eating Macaque
34275 Crab-eating Macaque
34276 Crab-eating Macaque
34278 Crab-eating Macaque
34279 Crab-eating Macaque
34280 Crab-eating Macaque
34281 Crab-eating Macaque
WNPRC
cj0233 Common Marmoset
cj0453 Common Marmoset D
cj0495 Common Marmoset
cj0506 Common Marmoset
cj1654 Common Marmoset
Piotr Rhesus Macaque
rhaf72 Rhesus Macaque
rhao45 Rhesus Macaque
Rh1890 Rhesus Macaque
R80180 Rhesus Macaque
R87083 Rhesus Macaque
R89124 Rhesus Macaque
R89163 Rhesus Macaque
R90128 Rhesus Macaque
R91040 Rhesus Macaque
R93014 Rhesus Macaque
S93052 Rhesus Macaque
R95054 Rhesus Macaque D
R95065 Rhesus Macaque D
R95076 Rhesus Macaque D
R95100 Rhesus Macaque
R96108 Rhesus Macaque
R97041 Rhesus Macaque
R97082 Rhesus Macaque
R97111 Rhesus Macaque
Response from Jordana Lenon, public relations manager for WNPRC. Citizens' requests Lenon refused to answer.
WANPRC
A03068 Rhesus Macaque
A98056 Pig-tailed Macaque
A92025 Baboon
F91396 Pig-tailed Macaque D
J90153 Pig-tailed Macaque
J90266 Pig-tailed Macaque
J90299 Crab-eating Macaque
J91076 Pig-tailed Macaque D
J91386 Pig-tailed Macaque D
J91398 Pig-tailed Macaque D
J92068 Pig-tailed Macaque
J92349 Pig-tailed Macaque D
J92476 Pig-tailed Macaque
UCLA
B15A Vervet
788E Rhesus Macaque
9382 Vervet
1984-016 Vervet
1991-016 Vervet
1992-015 Vervet
1994-014 Vervet
1994-046 Vervet
1994-087 Vervet
1995-046 Vervet
1995-101 Vervet
1996-022 Vervet
UTAH
MCY24525 Crab-eating Macaque
MCY24540 Crab-eating Macaque
OIPM-007 Crab-eating Macaque
MCY24525 Crab-eating Macaque
MCY24540 Crab-eating Macaque
UNC-Chapel Hill
3710 Squirrel Monkey
APF
Ashley Chimpanzee
Karla Chimpanzee
Tyson Chimpanzee
Snoy Chimpanzee
Maurice p1 Maurice p2 Chimpanzee
Hercules Chimpanzee
Jerome Chimpanzee
Ritchie Chimpanzee
Rex Chimpanzee
Topsey Chimpanzee
B.G. Chimpanzee
Dawn Chimpanzee
BamBam Chimpanzee
Dixie Chimpanzee
Ginger Chimpanzee
Kelly Chimpanzee
Lennie Chimpanzee
Kist Chimpanzee
Peg Chimpanzee
Aaron Chimpanzee
Chuck Chimpanzee
James Chimpanzee
Alex Chimpanzee
Muna Chimpanzee
Wally Chimpanzee
#1028 Chimpanzee
Lippy Chimpanzee
#1303 Chimpanzee
#CA0127 Chimpanzee
Shane Chimpanzee
LEMSIP
196 Baboon
The Fauna Foundation Chimpanzees
Center for Biologics Evaluation
Univ. of Alabama - Birmingham

Univ. of Minnesota

00FP8 Long-tailed Macaque
312E Rhesus Macaque
9711B Rhesus Macaque
99IP61 Long-tailed Macaque
CDC-Column E 2002

 

The National Institute of Child Health and Human Development

From its web site,

The National Institute of Child Health and Human Development is part of the National Institutes of Health, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. The NICHD conducts and supports laboratory, clinical and epidemiological research on the reproductive, neurobiologic, developmental, and behavioral processes that determine and maintain the health of children, adults, families and populations.

Mission Statement

The National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) seeks to assure that every individual is born healthy, is born wanted, and has the opportunity to fulfill his or her potential for a healthy and productive life unhampered by disease or disability.

Someone visiting the NICHD web page would probably go away thinking that this NIH agency is working on ways to improve the health of children. And to an extent, such an impression would be justified. Undoubtedly, there are many caring scientists funded by NICHD who are working long hours in honorable and compassionate endeavors. But, if you look a bit deeper you find this part of the NICHD web page:

Outside Inquires Into Your Animal Research

If you receive any inquires from Animal Welfare/Rights Organizations, the press, radio, television, or members of Congress, take their name(s) and telephone number and call the NICHD Office of Research Reporting (ORR) at 496-5133.

DO NOT provide any information before contacting ORR. [NICHD's emphasis.]

What could scientists associated with NICHD be up to that would make the agency worry about explaining how they are using animals? It is clear that scientists are justified when they give guarded answers to inquires from Animal Welfare/Rights Organizations, and, maybe, some media queries might bear a little careful planning before making a statement, but members of Congress?

What could scientists associated with NICHD be up to that would make the agency worry about explaining how they are using animals to members of Congress?

NICHD's intramural research is organized into four units. One of these is named the Laboratory of Comparative Ethology. It is under the direction of Stephen Suomi.

The American Heritage Dictionary defines ethology as the scientific study of animal behavior, usually as it occurs in a natural environment. The second definition given is 'the study of human ethos and its formation'.

Stephen Suomi was a graduate student at the University of Wisconsin; his teacher was Harry S. Harlow. The University of Wisconsin says, "Harlow's name is bonded to experiments that might be questionable today." You can sometimes hear members of the primate experimentation community say that Harlow's work is an artifact of bygone days when there were fewer safeguards in place. Today, strict oversight would not allow such clear cruelty to receive public funding.

Harry Harlow's experiments started out being only typically cruel. He discovered that an infant rhesus monkey will cling to a soft mannequin monkey rather than to a wire mannequin monkey even when the wire mannequin supplies the baby with milk. But Harlow's genius was that he could push the questions always further. His work would have been impossible without a supportive university willing to pay for his investigations. Harry and his students designed a variety of monkey mannequins. One was equipped with metal spines that could be quickly ejected and retracted. When an infant was clinging to its surrogate mother the scientists would eject the spines and the infant would be painfully pushed away from its "mother". They discovered that when the spines were withdrawn the baby would return and cling again. Another mannequin was designed with a series of tubes under the cloth covering that ice water could be circulated through. Again the baby was repelled by the "mother's" rejection but returned to cling again when the "mother" returned to normal.

In Harlow's darkest experiments he placed infants in V-shaped stainless steel boxes he termed the "wells of despair." When locked in these small vaults the infants were unable to see any other living creature including the person who replenished their food and water. And then he left the monkeys in these boxes for up to two years.

After they were removed from these prisons Harlow "discovered" that they had few social skills and seemed unable to interact with more normal monkeys. Harlow then impregnated some of these socially and emotionally crippled monkeys. When their babies were born Harlow documented all the many ways they killed their own children.

And this was the environment which nurtured Stephen Suomi, Head of the NICHD Laboratory of Comparative Ethology.

The introduction to his federally funded lab's web site says:

Research in this Laboratory is focused on behavioral, cognitive, and physiological development in humans and in nonhuman primates. The interactions of both genetic and environmental factors are explored in a comparative approach so as to determine the origins, ontogeny, and outcomes of various behavioral phenotypes.

Results of experimental studies in nonhuman primates are correlated with the results of longitudinal studies in human infants and their families, as well as with data obtained by physiological and molecular neuroscience techniques. Longitudinal designs are employed to address issues of developmental continuity and change; behavioral and physiological measures in support of multiple levels of analysis are carried out concomitantly.

So what's actually going on? The lab's web page says, The neuroanatomical, psychophysiological, and neurochemical concomitants of various behavioral phenotypes are tracked developmentally from birth through senescence in various rearing environments, including free-ranging outdoor settings. And there is nothing on the web page to suggest much else. Many scientists work in Suomi's lab. What follows is a brief survey of work they are involved in. It is important to keep in mind that this represents the part of their work they are willing to make public.

Suomi and Kathlyn Rasmussen, a member of his lab, recently presented a paper titled "Social Separation in Infant Cebus Apella: Patterns of Behavioral and Cortisol Response in which they describe the effects of separating infant capuchins from their mothers. They published another titled "Heart Rate Patterns in Rhesus Monkeys with Self-Injurious Behavior (SIB): Are These Monkeys "High Reactors?" in which isolated monkeys were intentionally frightened by someone with a net.

Monica Carosi works in Suomi's lab. She recently coauthored the paper "The Occurrence of a Bone-Like Structure in the Clitoris of Female Tufted Capuchins (Cebus Apella)," in which she noted that manually feeling for this structure is a better test than an x-ray.

Thomas Tsai and Maribeth Champoux also work with Suomi. They just presented "Early Social Rearing Environment Influences Acquisition of a Computerized Joystick Task in Rhesus Monkeys (Rhesus Macaque), and reported that [t]he long-lasting deleterious effects of early parental deprivation on rhesus monkeys' social and maternal behavior, physiology, and alcohol consumption are well-documented. By contrast, there is little evidence of the effect of early social rearing history on learning and cognition.

When lab claims that, various behavioral phenotypes are tracked developmentally from birth through senescence in various rearing environments, including free-ranging outdoor settings, it appears to be intentionally misleading. More accurately, for those involved in policy-making decisions such as Congresspersons, they shoul have said, ... including socially and envioronmentally deprived and stress-inducing settings.

Maribeth Champoux joined up with other Suomi lab researchers M. Purple, Rebecca Homer and Courtney Shannon-Lindell to write "Lack of Effect of Dietary DHA Supplementation on Behavioral Switches in Nursery-Reared Infant Rhesus Macaques." They say that some human children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder have low levels of the chemical DHA in their blood. So they fed some of the baby monkeys a normal diet, and some a diet high in DHA. They report that nothing seemed to happen.

Champoux, Homer, Shannon-Lindell, Purple and another lab member, K. Zajicek presented "The Impact of N-3 Fatty Acid Supplementation During Infancy on Later Social Behavior." They say that, Long chain essential fatty acids are . . . typically abundant in primate breast milk, and relatively deficient in standard infant formulas. It is unknown whether an infant diet low in long chain essential fatty acids has an effect on later behavior." They tested this question using 28 baby monkeys.

J. Dee Higley joined with lab members Rebecca Homer and Courtney Shannon-Lindell to present "CNS Monoamine Matabolites and Plasma Hormones as Predictors of Alcohol Consumption in Rhesus Macaques." They injected alcohol intravenously into 115 juvenile monkeys.

What would they say to a Congressperson's inquiry about this use of tax money?

Peggy O'Neill Wagner, another lab member, reported that vasectomized males in dominant positions in group housing continue to form consorts.

Lab members Thomas Tsai, Peter J. Pierre and J. Dee Higley wrote about monkeys being more fearful of a black plastic box than of a clear plastic box.

Kathlyn Rasmussen, of Suomi's lab, presented a paper in cooperation with scientists from Harvard's primate center titled "Reaction of Rhesus Monkeys with Self-Injurious Behavior to Heart Rate Testing: Is Biting a Coping Strategy?" They placed vests on monkeys known to bite themselves when placed in a vest and recorded their heart rates before, during, and after they bit themselves.

Gregory C. Westergaard, Liv Chanya, of Suomi's lab, and Suomi presented a paper on the willingness of capuchins to engage in barter, exchanging less desirable items for ones deemed to be of greater value.

[Studies referred to above are from the American Journal of Primatology, vol. 49, no. 1, 1999.]

Looking at these studies one is left still wondering what is being done at the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development that is so grotesque that it should be carefully packaged prior to disclosure to even a member of Congress. If they are willing to tell publicly about frightening young monkeys, what is it they wish to keep quiet about? Apparently, not even Congress will ever know for sure. Perhaps, its simply the fact that little of this has anything to do with human child health or human development.

Suomi, Westergaard, M.K. Izard, J.H. Drake, and J. Dee Higley published "Rhesus Macaque (Macaca mulata) Group Formation and Housing: Wounding and Reproduction in a Specific Pathogen Free (SPF) Colony" in 1999. They report that, when forming groups of monkeys, slow incremental additions to the group, with barriers that they are able to hide behind, results in less wounding than throwing a bunch of monkeys together in a single day, willy-nilly. This is particularly troubling because the most basic understanding of rhesus husbandry is their propensity to fight to establish dominance hierarchies. Forming a group all at once is unethical and malfeasant, yet this was part of this Suomi experiment.

In 1998, Suomi, along with other scientists at his lab wrote "Rearing Condition and Plasma Cortisol in Rhesus Monkey Infants" in which he continued to document the results of being raised in isolation.

In 1997 Suomi coauthored "Behavioral and Physiological Characteristics of Indian and Chinese-Indian Hybrid Rhesus Macaque Infants" which documented the effects of 4 weeks of the repetitive social-separation...

Suomi has been experimenting and publishing continually since at least 1970 when he published "Effect of Repetitive Infant-Infant Separation of Young Monkeys."
[Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1970 Oct;76(2):161-72.]

For 30 years Suomi has been experimenting on baby monkeys. His reward has been the directorship of the publicly funded National Institute of Child Health and Human Development's Laboratory of Comparative Ethology.

If you receive any inquires from Animal Welfare/Rights Organizations, the press, radio, television, or members of Congress, take their name(s) and telephone number and call the NICHD Office of Research Reporting (ORR) at 496-5133. DO NOT provide any information before contacting ORR.

NICHD

Rick Bogle, 1999


Primate Freedom Project
P.O. Box 1623
Fayetteville, GA. 30214
Tel: 678.489.7798

Email: info@primatefreedom.com


Home Page | Our Mission | News
What Are Primate Freedom Tags | Order Tag
Primate Research Centers | Resources